THE MISAPPLICATION OF VISUAL CRITERIA IN
HEARING AND
SOUND-REPRODUCTION
OR
OUR CHANGE IN ORIENTATION FROM SPACE AND
SOUND TO TIME
AND SIGHT
©1985 The Anstendig Institute
Revised
The
orientation of society towards time and space and their sensory corollaries,
seeing and hearing, has changed over the course of this century. Whereas people
used to be space and sound oriented, they have become time and sight oriented.
Instead of adding a heightened awareness of visual phenomena to mankind’s
previous orientation towards space-sound phenomena, the newly emphasized
awareness of visual phenomena has simply replaced the awareness of sound. The
obvious result has been a general lack of conscious awareness of sound. But it
has also resulted in the misuse of visual criteria in experiencing and
evaluating sound events, which, in turn, has fostered an insensitivity to the
important dynamic qualities of sound as a flow in time, i.e., expression and
nuance. Fostered by the deficiencies of early recorded sound and the
introduction of the static, dimensional effects of stereo, this emphasis on
visual criteria in processing sensory perceptions has been at the root of grave
errors in the development and evaluation of today’s systems for recording and
reproducing sound. The century-old faults in sound-reproduction (notably that
most sound-systems cannot reproduce the expressive qualities of sound) have
resulted in a general deterioration in musical performance--an emphasis of
technical proficiency over expressive values--and a lack of discrimination
among listeners.
Until
the development of clocks and watches, modern transportation and modern
timesaving machines in general, life was more leisurely in the sense that it
was not governed by firm time schedules: the crop was harvested until the work
was finished; meal preparation could not be judged to the minute and meals were
served when the food and the diners were ready; transportation was leisurely
and could not be kept to exact schedules. For most people, firm, minute-exact
appointments were rare occurrences, mainly public events, such as theater
performances, etc. But even public events assumed and allowed for late-comers:
all early operas, for example, had overtures, and composers saved the important
music until later, when the audience could be expected to have settled down.
Today,
firm, minute-exact time schedules rule people’s lives. Most people live
constantly under the pressure of appointments for which it is necessary to be
precisely on time. A good part of almost everyone’s life is spent rushing from
one such date to another, always under the pressure of a shortage of time. This
pressure of time was not known to former societies and has affected our
perception of sound.
It
takes time to hear sounds. The sounds must progress and develop over a period
of time before their important expressive and information-bearing qualities
become apparent. Also, anything that demands a high level of hearing acuity
usually takes even more time and demands quite a bit of physical calmness on
the part of the listener. In contrast, most of the visual phenomena in everyday
life can be assimilated almost instantly. Under pressures of time schedules,
the tendency is to quickly size up one’s surroundings visually. Since people
are now constantly in a hurry, there is only time to look where one is going
and not enough time to listen for the important qualities of one’s surroundings
that are contained in their sounds.
In
contemplating the orientation of the senses in relation to time and space, it
is necessary to bear in mind that, since time and space are polarities, they
are actually inseparable components of the same thing. We only distinguish the
one from the other for purposes of analysis in order to gain a deeper
understanding of the whole. The process is similar to analyzing a seesaw: we
differentiate the end going up from the end going down, but both ends are
inseparable parts of the same movement. Similarly, we distinguish the plus pole
from the minus pole of a magnet, but both poles are essential parts of a
magnet. Without both of them there would be no magnet.
Historically,
the most common symbol for the basic polarities has been the cross.1
Since a cross would not exist without both axes, the union of the vertical and
horizontal axes both differentiates the two poles and symbolizes their
indivisibility. In regard to time and space and seeing and hearing, the
horizontal axis symbolizes space and the vertical axis symbolizes time. Our
primary awareness of time has been attributed to the eyes and seeing because
one “sees” that time has elapsed by seeing changes in the space around us. Our
primary awareness of space is attributed to the ears and hearing because it is
sounds that convey a sense of depth and dimension in all directions around us.
Basically,
one sees only a flat, limited, frontal two- dimensionality, from which depth is
implied from experience. Everything that is seen in normal, everyday experience
can be placed by a painter or photographer on a two-dimensional surface, and
two-dimensional paintings and photographs can convey what is recognized as
depth, although they are, in reality, flat (tromp l’oeil
is the most famous example). The illusion of depth is also conveyed on the
flat, two-dimensional surface of a mirror. But one hears sound, which is
vibrating space, all around one. Since sound is heard from all directions, it
is intrinsically three-dimensional and cannot be represented two-dimensionally.
That
space and sound are all around us is symbolized by the horizontal line of the
cross. Because sight and time are only one-directional, frontal in the case of
sight and from the past through the present to the future in the case of time,
they are symbolized by the vertical line, which is thought of as moving through
the horizontal line of space. Both the new orientation towards time instead of
space and the validity of the symbolism of the cross as a representation of the
polarities is illustrated by modern architecture, in which the buildings now
rise upwards (the vertical axis of the cross) instead of spreading out in the
horizontal (the horizontal axis of the cross) as was the case when people were
space oriented (the pyramids of Egypt are prime examples of space-oriented
architecture).
While
it is true that the polarities time and space cannot exist without each other,
consciousness can be oriented more towards an awareness of the processes
associated with the one or the other. That is because the sensory processes of
seeing and hearing, which are involved in being aware of time and space
phenomena, and time and space themselves, are infinitely more complex than up
and down or plus and minus.
An
orientation towards space and sound is not detrimental to an awareness of time
and visual phenomena. But, for many reasons, an orientation towards time and
sight is detrimental to an awareness of the experiences inherent in the
space-sound phenomena and has a detrimental effect on the quality of life in general.
An important reason why an emphasized time-sight orientation is detrimental to
an awareness of sound is that it is easier to ignore sounds than it is to
ignore visual impressions. We continuously use sight to direct our movements
and also, since visual impressions can be selectively eliminated by looking
away or closing our eyes when we do not want to see them, there is a natural
tendency to take note of everything that is seen. But sounds are omnipresent.
Unlike the way we see, which is selective in regard to direction, we hear
sounds from all directions around us. Because sounds cannot be shut out by
closing or turning off the ears, we readily develop the habit of ignoring
sounds even though we hear them.
Hearing
has traditionally been recognized as the highest, most powerful of the two
higher senses because, when combined with sight, it determines the character of
the visual impressions and is capable of conveying the strongest, finest
quality, longest-sustained experiences. Hearing also contains the more purely
“human” factors in life because sounds are the means through which emotions and
emotional qualities are usually conveyed. Hearing must, therefore, be seen as
the most important factor in life, and an orientation towards time and sight, to
the detriment of an awareness of sound, must be seen as a grave impoverishment
of life.
To
fully understand time and space as it is experienced by the human being and
what is meant by being time or space oriented, it is necessary to understand a
seeming paradox in the way we experience time and space. Everything, when
reduced to what are generally accepted to be the smallest common denominators,
reduces to time and space. But the author would like to point out that the
reduction can and should be pursued even further, in which case, from the point
of view of human perception, space becomes the lowest common denominator and
time becomes only an adjunct of space. That is because, for the human being, an
awareness that time has passed results from being aware of progressive changes
in the space that we are conscious of: we know that time has passed only
because we are aware that the space around us has changed and from that we
imply that time has passed. However, the reverse is not true: when we sleep,
but do not dream, time elapses without causing any awareness of space. We can,
therefore, only be conscious of space, not time. If we do not experience an
awareness of space we have no awareness that time is passing. From our own
experience, we do not even have a way of ascertaining how much time has passed
without the aid of some form of clock, which, of course, is a space.
The
point I would like to emphasize (because it differs from most concepts of time)
is that everything that we are aware of is one form or another of space,
i.e., ever-changing space. Time is nothing more than a measure of
the progress of the changes in that space and a measure is just another form of
space. This concept merits detailed clarification. To rephrase it: space is the
thing itself, i.e., space is that which one is aware of, and anything that one
is aware of, including what is thought of as time itself, has to be a form of
space, not an increment of time.
Over
the ages, classic mystical-religious thought has colored and shaped most
civilized viewpoints of time and space. The religious viewpoint is that all of
the space of which our world consists (including past, present, and future)
exists simultaneously in the Creator’s consciousness, which is thought to be unlimited.
But human consciousness, being limited, can only be conscious of space along
the line from the past through the present to the future. Because time is thus
thought of as the progress of our consciousness of space from the past, through
the present, to the future, it is seldom realized that, in the way we actually
experience them, time is static while space is dynamic. In fact, most people
think of time as flowing while space is generally thought of as static. The
paradoxical reality is just the opposite: it is the space that is flowing in
our consciousness and our awareness of time occurs at isolated, static moments.
Although
time is a measure of the steadily progressing flow of space in our
consciousnesses, we can only be aware of time at given, static moments: by
comparing the impressions of space at these isolated moments we recognize that
the space has changed and, therefore, infer that time has gone by. On the other
hand, space is usually thought of as a static, solid “thing”. But that is misleading.
Sound, for example, is not solid, but definitely is a form of space. Since
space can only be experienced as a steady flow of space in our consciousness,
it is really our awareness of space that is a flowing thing, while our
awareness of time is not. In truth, it is not even an awareness of time; it is
simply a realization--a mentally arrived at conclusion--that time has gone by
based on the awareness that changes have occurred in the space around us.
To
comprehend space as flowing and our awareness of time as intermittent, it is
helpful to think of a well-known exercise in developing concentration: the discipline of looking steadily and unflaggingly at a candle.
In terms of time and space, the candle is a space which is steadily flowing in
time. It is not intermittent impressions (flashes) of a candle, but a steady
continuous, unceasing flow of the candle that takes place in the viewer’s
consciousness. In other words, it is the space (the candle), with all of its
characteristics and qualities, that is flowing, not time. It follows that, for
a human being, time is not experienced and, therefore, possesses no qualities
in itself. Any qualities attributed to time (that it
is passing slowly or quickly, that it is boring, etc.) are really qualities of
the space being experienced and not qualities of time itself, even if that
space is just emptiness. But space can have many qualities that are essentially
independent of time. It is true that these qualities of space, even those that
occur in a split instant, must be experienced in time, i.e., over a period of
time, however short. But it is the space and its passage in time, not in any
way time itself, that possesses the qualities.
Similarly,
visual impressions, like time, do not, in themselves
contain emotional (or other) qualities. The emotions that we experience in
relation to visual images are mentally supplied in relation to past experience
or anticipated effect. For example, what is seen as tragic or frightening by
one person can be completely neutral or even funny to another person unfamiliar
with the experiences associated with that image. But sounds in themselves
contain the emotional qualities that people experience from hearing them. Good
music, for example, contains harmonic, rhythmic, and melodic vibrational
patterns with which the body vibrates in sympathy (like the sounding board of a
musical instrument, only much more complex). The music actually causes the body
to vibrate in the emotional patterns of the music and a new emotion can
actually be experienced through the music itself. If music is played along with
a visual image, the experience of the combined musical and visual experience
will essentially contain the emotion of the music, not that of the image: a
happy scene with sad music will result in a sad experience; a sad scene with
happy music will not result in a sad experience, etc. (The examples are
oversimplified to make the point. Some elements of visual images such as color
and brightness do directly affect us and influence the combined audio-visual
experience, but much less strongly).
Since
we only infer time from an awareness that space has changed, a precise
awareness of time is impossible except by taking isolated instants in the
progression of ever-changing space and measuring the intervals between those
instants by comparing them to a space that is changing in a constant, known
manner, i.e., a clock, sun dial, heart-beat, etc. It is important to comprehend
1) that a human being experiences space, not time, 2) that the fact that time has
elapsed is only recognized, not experienced, at static, isolated moments, and
3) that we human beings only recognize that time has passed because we
experience changes in the space that we were aware of (if we could sleep for 8
hours without dreaming, when we woke up, it could just as well have been 1
minute, because we were not aware of any space during that time).2
Direct
comparison of unchanging subjects at given, isolated moments in time is the
analytical method of the time-oriented person. A time-oriented person will
preserve isolated impressions of space at given moments in time and study them,
usually visually, by direct comparison, i.e., by placing them next to each
other. The characteristic of all such studies is that the object of study is static,
at least at the moment it is studied (the object may change or deteriorate over
a long period of time, but, essentially, at the moment it is compared and
studied, it is static). Of all analytical processes that use sensory
perceptions, direct comparison, which is only possible with sight, is the most
precise and the most comfortable because it does not place demands on our
memory.
The
study and evaluation of sound, however, does not allow for such direct
comparison because sounds, being a flow of space in time, do not remain still
to be compared directly to each other. Most of the important contents of sounds
themselves are contained in the manner in which the sounds flow in time, i.e.,
the expression, inflection, nuance, etc., which occur as the connected sounds
progress over a period of time. These expressive possibilities, which are
intrinsic only to sound, contain and convey most of the human emotional content
of life itself. They are more powerful even than the meanings contained in
words or other informational sonic symbols.3 The expression with
which words are pronounced, for example, will determine and even change the
meaning conveyed by those words. Isolated sounds (e.g., single, isolated
instances of everyday sounds like a split instant of a baby’s crying or a motor
starting, and single notes, chords, or instrumental colorings in music, etc.)
do not have any significance or communicate anything meaningful except in
relation to that which precedes and follows them, even if what precedes and follows
them is silence.
Unfortunately,
society’s change in orientation from space and hearing to time and sight has
resulted in its wrongly applying the criteria and procedures for evaluating
visual phenomena to the evaluation of sound. This misapplication of visual
criteria to the experience and even the study of sound is the cause of the
already lamented loss of awareness of important aspects of sound. Since sound
is a progression of constantly changing space in time, certain aspects of the
experiences that can be had from sound which are caused by the progress of
sounds over a period of time are intrinsic only to sound. These special
phenomena are the expressive-emotional experiences as well as the communication
of meanings and subtleties beyond the power of words. It is the effects that
result from sound progressing in time (the quality of attack and release of
notes, the inflections, the subtle dynamic fluctuations, in other words, the
expressive nuances) that are most intrinsic to the sound experience, that set
it off from other experiential phenomena, and that contain the essence of what
mankind seeks in the sound experience, particularly in music, but also in
speech and other sonic communication. The loss of awareness of these factors
has had particularly deplorable ramifications in the field of
sound-reproduction.
In
evaluating the accuracy of the various means of recording and playing back
sound (records, tapes, CD and other digital recordings, sound-systems, etc.),
the emphasis has been mistakenly placed on static factors that do not take time
to hear. Those factors have nothing to do with the flow of sounds in time,
i.e., they have nothing to do with the expressive, musical content of the
sounds. Little is said about the factors in the progress of the music which
determine real musical culture, i.e., the quality of attacks and releases, the
subtlety of, and lack of vulgarity in, the phrasing, even though these factors,
especially the phrasing of the melodies, are the most important aspects of musical
performance. People no longer listen for these aspects of sound.
The
major factors that are mentioned in evaluations of sound- reproduction are
static spatial dimensions such as the “sound-stage” (the height, width and
depth of the “stereo image”), distortions in the coloring of the sounds,
“gritty” highs, too little bass, too much treble, etc. These factors are all
essentially static factors usually associated with sight, which can be
determined by analyzing single isolated moments in the progress of the sounds.
They have little to do with the important expressive or informational contents
of sound. In fact, any of those static factors can be changed quite a bit
without ruining the expressive or informational content. For example, the
expression of music comes through just as well in monophonic recordings as in
stereophonic recordings and, while instrumental colorings can enhance music,
changing them somewhat does not change the expressive content unless the new
colorings add irritations that disturb the listener. In real life, no two
instruments sound the same and each instrument sounds different in each room in
which it is played. It takes quite a bit of distortion or introduction of
irritations to disturb the expressive content, if that expressive content has
been reproduced (unfortunately, irritations, which can be eliminated, do occur
in sound-reproduction and most sound- reproduction does not reproduce all the
nuances of the flow of sound in time4).
In
sound-reproduction, the misapplication of visual criteria to sonic phenomena
has resulted in such unfortunate developments as the introduction of Stereo
(the two-channel attempt to reproduce static spatial dimensions) before sound
reproduction via a single channel had been perfected, and the introduction and
almost universal acceptance of a not yet perfected form of digital recording
that cannot accurately preserve dynamic subtleties.5
The
introduction and acceptance of the present digital recording systems is proof
that the deterioration in the awareness of expressive nuance has already
progressed to the point where people no longer listen for or care about fine
expressive nuance. In analog recordings, the dynamic qualities were at least
preserved in the recording process. It was the retrieval of this information by
most playback systems that was inadequate. But, with the digital recording
system that has now been adopted by the whole recording industry, the subtle
dynamic information can never be retrieved because it is simply not captured in
the recording. Yet, despite much publicity about the shortcomings of digital,
the public does not miss those finer nuances and digital is having a big
success.
Regarding
the introduction of Stereo, since human consciousness is limited, there is a
limit to how many different things a human being can be simultaneously aware
of. In fact, most people can only concentrate fully on one thing at a time.
Placing an emphasis on an awareness of static effects of spatial dimensions has
to detract from an awareness of the expression and the development of
sensitivity to fine nuance. That is exactly what happened when Stereo was
introduced. Since sensitivity to delicate, exquisite nuances of sound is an
epitome of sensory experience, this mistaken emphasis on static, dimensional
aspects of sound was a serious tragedy for mankind that is reflected in all
aspects of modern life, but particularly in the the
impersonal quality of modern life and in the quality of current musical
interpretation. At that time, playback equipment had not yet been able to
reproduce the finer nuances of the great recorded performances. The addition of
a slew of new, static stereo effects to listen for cemented an already
well-developed insensitivity to the finer, more delicate aspects of sound,
which contain the most strongly moving and emotionally meaningful aspects of
the sound experience.
Further
reasons why the public has never become aware of the lack of expressive detail
in the playback of its recordings are because 1) it has no way of knowing what
subtleties the original performances contained, 2) some expression is heard and
experienced, albeit a changed, degraded, coarser, less meaningful expression,
3) the static, spatial aspects of sound are much easier to hear than subtleties
of expressive nuance, 4) the static effects are superficially entertaining,
especially when one is not aware the fine points of the music are missing, 5)
the music itself is usually sufficiently robust to retain some appeal without
expression or with the wrong expression, and 6) since the public has become
time-sight oriented and expression and emotions belong more to the realm of
space and hearing, mankind’s tendency to be aware of and seek out, experience,
and evaluate the emotional contents of things in general has been greatly
weakened. Society has been conditioned not to hear subtle nuances.6
Of course, emotions are still experienced, i.e., through films and TV, but the
critical, evaluative tendencies of society are directed towards the
visual-informational aspects of experiences.7
Unfortunately,
the performers and music students have also grown up with the deficiencies of
sound-reproduction. Like the rest of the public, they listen to recordings and
are peppered with recorded background music wherever they go. This, along with
the general orientation towards visual aspects of life, has resulted in a deterioration
in interpretive insights and subtleties, a vulgarization of expressive content,
and a general interpretive crisis in the classical music field which serious,
knowledgeable, older musicians have been aware of for decades. For decades, the
emphasis of superficial technical wizardry at the expense of expression has
been epidemic and is now firmly ensconced in the musical scene. All the better
music teachers currently complain that there is enormous technical talent in
the schools but very few of these technically proficient students have anything
to express. Possibly the worst result of the new time-sight orientation is the
now universal use of bad, vulgar, expressionless background music. This
uninvolved, either emotionally dead or emotionally vulgar, music has acted as a
sedative to a public sensitivity already crippled by the deficiencies of
recorded sound.
A
disturbing result of the shift in orientation from sound to visual phenomena is
the introduction of visual distractions to the experience of music. Most people
have become uncomfortable with and unable to enjoy music unless they have
something interesting to look at. Unfortunately, since human consciousness is
limited, music itself almost always consists of more than the amount of space
that most people can be simultaneously conscious of. There are all the voices
and all the notes of a chordal accompaniment plus
such details as the expression with which each voice is played or sung, which
include minute nuances of tempo and dynamic fluctuations. Introducing visual
elements has to distract the listener from some part or other of the music.
The
Anstendig Institute has many opportunities to observe the effects of visual
distractions on listening. The institute’s programs of recorded music and its
other activities in the field of sound-reproduction place the listeners in the
situation where there is finely nuanced music to listen to and nothing to look
at. We have had the opportunity, both in our public programs and our research
sessions, to observe that reading while listening disturbs and even ruins the
experience of the expressive content of music. The experience of a recording of
the Virgil Thomson, Gertrude Stein opera Four Saints In Three Acts was
compromised by passing out librettos. The audience read instead of listening
and experiencing, and the experience of that music did
not happen as it did with other audiences that just listened without reading. At a program of a recording of the Mahler Third Symphony, a
gentleman who brought a score to follow and, at the author’s request, consented
not to do so, left early in the performance, evidently not entertained enough
by merely listening.
Probably
the most subtle problem relating to visual distractions in music is whether or
not to use subtitle-translations projected above the stage during opera
performances. This question is still being hotly argued in the musical world.
But the fact that those in favor of subtitles are winning and subtitles are
being used in more and more productions is further proof of the new visual
orientation of society. A learned, well considered decision on this subject
demands a clear understanding of why a disproportionate time-sight orientation
is detrimental to the awareness of sound, particularly such complex sounds as
those of music. Undeniably, an awareness of exactly what is going on in the
action is helpful in experiencing the musical content. But just as undeniably,
our institute’s experience has shown that the subtitles can be expected to
distract from some musical values. They also can keep the audience from sinking
as deeply as possible into the musical expression, because any unnecessary
physical tension detracts from the ability to experience sensory stimulae.
The
more relaxed the body is, the more perceptively one is able to hear and the
more intense the inner sensations, i.e., the emotional experience, can be (the
same is true of all sensory perceptions, whether it be the tasting of wine, or
the experience of intense sexual sensations). The muscles around the eyes are
extremely critical tension-centers for the whole body.8 Their
various states of tension and relaxation affect many other parts of the body.
It is also well-known that we are most sensitively aware when the eyes are
centered in their sockets, without any tension in the muscles that move them to
one side or the other of their sockets (this is the main reason for the
practice of meditating with the eyes closed and centered in their sockets. The
muscles of the eyes are then most relaxed).
The
tensions caused by moving the eyes and even the neck-muscles to read subtitles,
creates a tension and distraction that must have an effect on the experience of
the music. The question is whether that is worse than the effects of being
ignorant of the story. From long experience with this problem, we at The
Anstendig Institute are convinced that visual distractions of any kind,
including subtitles, should be avoided, or so organized that a minimum of
tension (activity of the eye muscles) is necessary to read them. That means
that the placement of the subtitles is of crucial importance and that they
should not be placed far from the action itself. While such subtitles can
easily be placed next to the action on a TV screen and the small size of the TV
screen makes possible viewing them without moving one’s eyes, a large opera
stage and even a large movie-screen presents great problems. There is the very
real risk that the tensions caused by the muscles moving the eyes will cause
further tensions that will ruin the intensity of the collective
artistic-emotional audience experience, in which everyone settles down and
seems to hear and experience as one.
As
the human being experiences it, the world we live in is a space, of which time
is merely an adjunct. The world is, therefore, a continuum, a progress,
a continuing development of space in our consciousnesses, the measure of which
is time. Because hearing is the dominant, most powerfully affecting of the
senses and only space, not time, possesses qualities, an awareness of space
and, along with it, an awareness of sound should have
priority over time and sight. In reality, society did first develop an
orientation towards an awareness of space and sound. It follows that the
development of mankind’s awareness and orientation towards time-visual
phenomena should have been an addition to its already developed awareness of
space-sound phenomena as a further enrichment of life, and not, as has
happened, at the expense of society’s awareness and appreciation of the sound experience
and those special phenomena intrinsic to sound. The world has already been
immensely impoverished by the present imbalance towards visual orientation. The
situation should be comprehended and the imbalance in sensory perception
corrected if mankind is to achieve a meaningful quality of life in the modern
world.
1 Our
paper, “Hearing; The Informational and The Experiential” lists the classic
polarities as they relate to time and space and the vertical and horizontal
axes of the cross.
2 It
is possible that some readers might feel that quantum mechanics and certain
modern scientific theories conflict with some of the ideas set forth in these
arguments. But this paper deals solely with how the human being experiences
time and space. For example, science tells us that the apparently simultaneous
movement of the opposite ends of a seemingly rigid see-saw is not really
simultaneous; that the end opposite the leverage actually moves a little bit
later than the end where the leverage is applied. But that scientifically
ascertained fact does not in any way alter the human being’s perception that
the two sides of the see-saw move simultaneously.
3 0ur
paper, “Hearing; The Informational and The Experiential”, explains the
different types of sonic content.
4 For
detailed information on the problems of accurate sound reproduction we refer
the reader to The Anstendig Institute’s papers on sound reproduction, sound
equalization and digital recording.
5 The
Anstendig Institute papers on digital recordings explain why current digital
recording is flawed and cannot be perfected until the whole industry adapts a
new norm with a vastly higher sampling rate.
6 Our
paper, “Hearing; Our Conditioned Responses to Music”, discusses how we can
become conditioned to hear music in set ways.
7 Our
paper, “Hearing; The Informational and The Experiential”, explains the
different, experiential and informational aspects of hearing and relates them
to the classic polarities.
8 The
clinically well-documented Auto-genic training of Dr.
Johannes Schulz, shows that the muscles that control
the eyes are major tension centers in that relaxing them causes a sympathetic
reaction in numerous other muscles of the body, which relax down in sympathy
with the relaxation of tension in the eye muscles.
The
Anstendig Institute is a non-profit, tax-exempt, research institute that was
founded to investigate stress-producing vibrational influences in our lives and
to pursue research in the fields of sight and sound; to provide material
designed to help the public become aware of and understand stressful
vibrational influences; to instruct the public in how to improve the quality of
vibrational influences in their lives; and to provide research and explanations
for a practical understanding of the psychology of seeing and hearing.